Tuning Fork Tests
Tuning
forks were used to test hearing long before the development of the audiometer.
Schwabach Test
The Schwabach
test is a technique for estimating a patient’s hearing sensitivity by
bone-conduction. The test has two principal characteristics:
1.
It makes use
of the fact that the tone produced by a tuning fork becomes softer with time
after it has been struck due to damping, and
2.
The
patient’s hearing is expressed in relative terms compared with the examiner’s
hearing ability. This comparison is done by timing how long the tuning fork is
heard by the patient and how long it is heard by the examiner. The basic
procedure involves placing the base of the vibrating fork on the patient’s
mastoid process until the tone fades away. The clinician then moves the fork to
his own mastoid and times how long he can hear it. Compared with how long the
examiner can hear the tone, it is expected that the patient will hear the tone
o
For
a shorter period of time if she has a sensorineural loss;
o
For
a longer period of time (or perhaps the same length of time) if she has a
conductive loss; and
3.
For
the same amount of time if she has normal hearing. Schwabach outcomes are
problematic when dealing with mixed losses. The Schwabach test provides a
relative estimate of hearing at best, and its validity is completely dependent
on the tenuous assumption that the examiner really has normal hearing. It is
not surprising this test is rarely if ever used.
Weber
Test
The Weber test is
used to help determine whether a unilateral hearing loss is sensorineural or
conductive.
It is a lateralization
test because the patient is asked to indicate the direction from which a
sound
appears to be coming.
Before starting this test, the patient should be advised that it is possible for the
tone to be heard from
the good side or the poorer side,
or any other location for that matter. The procedure involves putting the base
of the vibrating tuning fork somewhere on the midline of the skull, most commonly
on the center of the forehead or the top of the head. The audiometric Weber
test uses the bone conduction vibrator instead of tuning forks. The patient is
asked to indicate where the
tone is heard. Hearing the tone in the better
ear implies that there is a sensorineural
loss in the poorer ear, whereas hearing the tone in the poorer ear suggests a conductive loss in that ear. The tone
is heard in the middle of the head, “all over,” or equally in both ears when
the patient has normal hearing, although some patients with sensorineural
losses also report such midline lateralizations. If there is a mixed loss, the
tone will be lateralized to the better ear if its level is below thepoorer ear’s
bone-conduction threshold. The Weber test will fail to detect the conductive
component of a mixed loss in such cases. The Weber test works for several
reasons, all of which are related to the idea that the bone-conduction tone
from the tuning fork reaches both cochleae at the same intensity.
The tone lateralizes to the better ear with sensorineural
losses for either of two reasons:
1.
The
tone will only be heard in the better ear if its level is lower than the
bone-conduction threshold of the poorer ear.
2.
The
second mechanism is due to the Stenger effect, which means that a sound presented
to both ears is perceived only in the ear where it is louder. The intensity of
the tone from the tuning fork will have a higher sensation level in the better
ear than in the impaired ear. Hence, it will be louder in the better ear and
will be perceived there. Several factors can explain why a bone-conduction tone
would be louder in (and thus lateralized to) the poorer ear. These mechanisms are just briefly mentioned
because they are beyond the scope of an introductory text:
a.
Outer
ear obstructions (e.g., impacted cerumen) may cause an occlusion effect,
b.
Mass
loading of the middle ear system caused by effusions or ossicular chain
interruptions may lower its resonance, and
c.
Phase
advances may be caused by fixations or interruptions of the ossicular chain.
Bing
Test
The Bing test is used
to determine if closing off the patient’s ear canal results in an occlusion
effect. The audiometric version of the test has already been discussed. In the
traditional Bing test the patient is asked to report whether a tuning fork
sounds louder with the ear canal open or closed. The base of a vibrating tuning
fork is held against the patient’s mastoid process. The tester then presses the
tragus down over the entrance of the ear canal to occlude it. The usual
technique is to alternately occlude and unocclude the ear canal to help the
patient make a reliable louder/softer judgment. It is desirable to make sure
that the tuning fork sounds louder when the ear is closed and softer when the
ear is open, instead of just asking whether the tone pulses between louder and
softer. Unlike the audiometric version, which involves thresholds and thus
quantifies the amount of the occlusion effect, the outcome of the tuning fork
Bing test is based completely on a subjective judgment of louder versus not
louder. If the occlusion effect is present, covering the ear canal should cause
the tuning fork to sound louder. This is called a positive result and implies
that the ear is either normal or has a sensorineural hearing loss. A negative
result occurs if closing off the ear canal fails to make the tuning fork sound
louder, and implies that there is either a conductive or mixed hearing loss.
Rinne
Test
The Rinne test is a
tuning fork procedure that compares hearing by air-conduction and by
bone-conduction; however, the approach used is different from the one used in
pure tone audiometry. The Rinne test is based on the idea that the hearing
mechanism is normally more efficient by air-conduction than it is by
bone-conduction. For this reason, a tuning fork will sound louder by
air-conduction than by bone conduction. However, this air-conduction advantage is
lost when there is a conductive hearing loss, in which case the tuning fork
sounds louder by bone conduction than by air-conduction.
Administering the
Rinne test involves asking the patient to indicate whether a vibrating tuning
fork sounds louder when its base is held against the mastoid process (bone conduction)
or when its prongs are held near the pinna, facing the opening of the ear canal
(air-conduction). After striking the fork, the clinician alternates it between
these two positions so that the patient can make a judgment about which one is
louder. The bone-conduction vibrator is used instead of the tuning fork in the
audiometric version of the Rinne test, and the patient indicates whether the
vibrator sounds louder on the mastoid or in front of the ear canal. Masking
noise must be put into the opposite ear to make sure that the Rinne results are
really coming from the test ear.
The outcome of the
Rinne test is traditionally called “positive” if the fork is louder by
air-conduction, and this finding implies that the ear is normal or has a
sensorineural hearing loss. The results are
called “negative” if
bone-conduction is louder than air-conduction, which is interpreted as
revealing the presence of a conductive abnormality. This terminology is
confusing because the examiner is often concerned with identifying a conductive
loss with this test. Consequently, many clinicians prefer to describe Rinne
results as “air better than bone” (AC > BC) versus “bone better than air”
(BC > AC). In these terms, AC > BC implies normal hearing or
sensorineural impairment, and BC > AC implies a conductive disorder.
Sometimes, the air
and bone-conduction signals sound equally loud to the patient (AC = BC). This equivocal
outcome can usually be overcome by usingthe timed Rinne test (Gelfand 1977).
This more accurate way to administer the Rinne test involves timing how long the patient can hear
the tuning fork at the two locations.
In this case, the
results are Positive (AC > BC) when the tone is heard longer by air-conduction,
and
a.
Negative
(BC > AC) when it is heard longer by bone-conduction.
Another variation of
the timed Rinne test involves holding the tuning fork at the mastoid until the
tone has faded away, and then moving it to the ear canal (and vice versa). Here, the result is
a.
AC
> BC if the tone can still be heard by air-conduction after it faded away by
boneconduction, and
b.
BC
> AC if the tone can still be heard by bone-conduction after it faded away
by air conduction.
Some
Comments on Tuning Fork Tests
Tuning fork tests are
quick and easy to administer and do not require special instrumentation, so
they
can provide general,
on-the-spot clinical insights, and they provide
important (albeit limited) diagnostic information, especially when an audiogram
is not available. However, tuning fork tests fall short of audiological
measures in their ability to assess the patient’s hearing status. This is due
to the greater
precision
of audiometric tests made possible by calibrated electronic equipment and
systematic testing strategies. Moreover, tuning fork tests are subject to considerable
variability in administration and subjectivity in interpretation. Several
limitations have already been mentioned with respect to individual tests, and
others are worthy of mention. Which frequencies are tested varies among
clinicians; some test only at 512 Hz and others use various combinations of
frequencies. The intensity of the tone produced by the tuning fork depends on
how hard it is struck each time, which causes stimulus levels to be inconsistent.
Subjective patient responses can be a confounding variable, especially when
dealing with younger children, and when the perception is “not logical” (e.g.,
when a tone is heard in the bad ear or gets louder when the ear is closed off).
Also, tuning fork tests are usually done in examination rooms and clinics that
are not sound isolated. Hence, noises that can mask the test signal and/or
distract the patient are often a real issue.
It is
therefore not surprising that carefully done
studies
have shown that tuning forks are less accurate than audiometric methods. Wilson
and Woods
(1975)
found that both the Bing and Rinne tests failed to achieve a high level of
accuracy in properly
identifying
conductive versus nonconductive losses. Gelfand (1977) studied the diagnostic
accuracy of the Rinne test. All of the tuning fork tests were done with masking
of the opposite ear. He found that the Rinne test cannot identify a conductive
loss with reasonable accuracy until the size of the air-bonegap becomes at
least 25 to 40 dB wide. Thus,
mild conductive hearing losses that are easily revealed audiometrically are
frequently missed by tuning fork tests like the Rinne. Some tuning fork test
problems are exacerbated by how and where the test is done. For example, the
need for masking with certain tuning fork tests, particularly the Rinne.
However, few physicians actually perform the Rinne test this way.
No comments:
Post a Comment